Abstract
Understanding what constrains signalling and maintains signal honesty is a central theme in animal communication. Clear cases of dishonest signalling, and the conditions under which they are used, represent an important avenue for improved understanding of animal communication systems. Female mimicry, when certain males take on the appearance of females, is most commonly a male alternative reproductive tactic that is condition-dependent. A number of adaptive explanations for female mimicry have been proposed including avoiding the costs of aggression, gaining an advantage in combat, sneaking copulations with females on the territories of other males, gaining physiological benefits and minimizing the risk of predation. Previous studies of female mimicry have focused on a single mode of communication, although most animals communicate using multiple signals. Male Augrabies flat lizards adopt alternative reproductive tactics in which some males (she-males) mimic the visual appearance of females. We experimentally tested in a wild population whether she-males are able to mimic females using both visual and chemical signals. We tested chemical recognition in the field by removing scent and relabelling females and she-males with either male or female scent. At a distance, typical males (he-males) could not distinguish she-males from females using visual signals, but during close encounters, he-males correctly determined the gender of she-males using chemical signals. She-males are therefore able to deceive he-males using visual but not chemical signals. To effectively deceive he-males, she-males avoid close contact with he-males during which chemical cues would reveal their deceit. This strategy is probably adaptive, because he-males are aggressive and territorial; by mimicking females, she-males are able to move about freely and gain access to females on the territories of resident males.
Keywords: female mimicry, multiple signals, lizard, chemical signal, visual signal, signal deception
1. Introduction
Classical sexual selection systems, where males are elaborately ornamented and territorial, are characterized by intense male rivalry (Andersson 1994; Andersson & Iwasa 1996). In these systems, fighting may be costly and individuals are at risk of physical injury (Clutton-Brock et al. 1979). For young males with little fighting experience, the risk of injury may be high because the disparity in fighting ability with older and larger males is often substantial (Cooper & Vitt 1987). One solution is to adopt an alternative reproductive tactic (Gross 1996) such as being a non-territorial ‘floater’, which reduces conflict with rival territorial males (Sinervo & Lively 1996), or to mask their true identity, such as in the case of female mimicry (Burley 1982; Slagsvold & Saetre 1991; Saetre & Slagsvold 1996; Sinervo & Lively 1996; Shine et al. 2001). Female mimicry takes many different forms, but can simply involve delaying maturation and retaining a female-like appearance until an individual is better able to deal with rival aggression (Martin & Forsman 1999). For example, some male birds delay plumage maturation to avoid harassment from aggressive rival males or because it gives them a competitive advantage in contests (Saetre & Slagsvold 1996), while the lizard Psammodromus algirus delays the development of head colour, which would normally invite attack from other males (Martin & Forsman 1999). By mimicking a female, males are often able to gain access to females that are guarded by a resident male or on his territory (Dominey 1980; Zamudio & Sinervo 2000; Hanlon et al. 2005). Deceptive signalling may therefore have the dual role of reducing directed aggression or harassment and allowing access to females that may reside on a resident male's territory.
Although the proximate function of a signal is to manipulate a receiver, signalling systems are thought to break down if, on average, signals are not honest (Maynard Smith & Harper 2003). How signals are constrained and how cheating is prevented is a central theme in animal communication and has attracted considerable recent attention (Johnstone 1995a,b; Zahavi & Zahavi 1997; Maynard Smith & Harper 2003; Whiting et al. 2003). We now have a relatively extensive list of examples of honest signals, verified through experimental manipulation of the signal. For example, threat posturing in side-blotched lizards is energetically expensive and can only be maintained by individuals in good condition (Brandt 2003). Our understanding of dishonest signals is also growing (e.g. Adams & Caldwell 1990; Møller 1990; Backwell et al. 2000; Hughes 2000; Briffa 2006). In the context of sexual selection, female mimicry dishonestly signals gender and can be adaptive for a certain class of males (Peschke 1987; Shine et al. 2001; Hanlon et al. 2005). One of the most spectacular examples of female mimicry occurs in the giant Australian cuttlefish sepia apama. Male giant cuttlefish are able to instantaneously mimic female colour, posture and behaviour, using neural control, at a rate of change of approximately 10 times per 15 min (Hanlon et al. 2005). While most examples of female mimicry involve visual signals, chemical mimicry of females by males can also occur (Mason & Crews 1985; Peschke 1987; Forsyth & Alcock 1990). Some male garter snakes Thamnophis sirtalis parietalis mimic female skin lipids (Mason & Crews 1985; Shine et al. 2000) when they emerge from hibernation, which attracts misdirected courtship from other males. This mimicry allows she-males to heat up faster in mating balls and protects them from avian predators, but its benefits are short-lived; typically, she-males only mimic female pheromones for a few days after emergence from hibernation (Shine et al. 2001). Therefore, the traditional hypothesis that female mimicry is sexually selected is not supported for garter snakes.
Because signal efficacy depends on both the signalling environment and the receiver's sensory system (Endler 1992, 1993; Johnstone 1997), most animals use multiple signals that may be of a visual, chemical or auditory nature (Hughes 1996; Rowe 1999; Maynard Smith & Harper 2003). For example, in lizards, visual signals are thought to be important at a distance while chemical signals are more important during close encounters (Lopez & Martin 2001; Lopez et al. 2002). Chemical and visual signals can also interact in a more complex way such that certain species that have less elaborate coloration may be more prone to using chemical signals (Hews & Benard 2001). While female mimicry of visual signals and/or behaviour has been documented for a number of lizard species (Cooper & Greenberg 1992; Wikelski et al. 1996; Zamudio & Sinervo 2000), chemical mimicry is largely unknown, the garter snake T. s. parietalis notwithstanding (Mason & Crews 1985; Shine et al. 2000). In garter snakes, males use both visual and chemical signals to select mates (Shine & Mason 2001). Recent reviews of signalling have emphasized the importance of understanding the role of multiple signals in animal communication and, in particular, how receivers deal with multiple signals (Johnstone 1995a, 1996, 1997; Rowe 1999; Andersson et al. 2002). While several studies have examined the mimicry of a single type of signal, we know of no study that has directly tested whether a single species is capable of producing multiple dishonest signals and how these signals may interact in the context of sexual selection.
Augrabies flat lizards (Platysaurus broadleyi) present a good system for examining the role of deceptive signalling because some adult males retain a female-like appearance and delay the onset of adult male coloration, even though they are sexually mature. Furthermore, they use multiple signals in a social context (M. J. Whiting 1995, personal observation) and adopt male alternative reproductive tactics (Whiting et al. 2006). At a distance, males use visual signals to evaluate a conspecific's gender (Whiting & Bateman 1999) or a male's fighting ability (Whiting et al. 2006). During close encounters, males use tongue-flicking to obtain chemical cues. Most male–male contests are settled using a visual signal, the UV reflective throat (Whiting et al. 2006), which acts as an honest signal of status (Stapley & Whiting 2006). In the case of females, males use visual signals for initial gender recognition and thereafter approach the females using a stereotypical courtship display (Whiting & Bateman 1999). If females allow close enough approach, males will then tongue-flick the female's flanks and/or cloacal region (M. J. Whiting 1995, personal observation), presumably to assess reproductive status. The length of time young males (hereafter she-males) retain female-like coloration is probably a condition-dependent strategy because all males ultimately develop typical male (hereafter he-male) coloration. That is, because time to adult colour expression is variable, the switch point at which she-males become he-males could potentially be dependent on prevailing social conditions (e.g. presence and number of adult males). Males are brightly coloured, territorial and highly aggressive (Whiting 1999; Whiting et al. 2003, 2006). Females are drab brown and have dorsal stripes (Branch & Whiting 1997). As juveniles, males appear female-like and initial colour development occurs on the venter, where it is concealed. During this time, he-males may visually mistake she-males for females and may court them (M. J. Whiting, J. K. Webb & J. S. Keogh 2003, personal observation). However, once the colour begins spreading to the sides and dorsum, these males are chased and attacked by he-males (M. J. Whiting, J. K. Webb & J. S. Keogh 2003, personal observation). Examples of she-males, females and typical males are depicted in figure 1.
Figure 1.
Examples of flat lizards (P. broadleyi). (a) Lizards in (i) could be either female or she-male, (ii) is a typical male and (iii) are typical females. (b) Ventral views are presented for (i) female, (ii) male and (iii)–(iv) she-male flat lizards.
In this study, we investigated whether she-males can manipulate multiple signals to deceive a receiver. Specifically, we tested whether she-males conceal their identity from he-males using both visual and chemical (scent) signals. Finally, we examined the function and behavioural costs associated with dishonest signals. All of our experiments were performed in the wild.
2. Material and methods
(a) Study system
We conducted fieldwork during September 2003 at Augrabies Falls National Park (hereafter Augrabies; 28° 35′ S, 20° 20′ E), Northern Cape Province, South Africa. Flat lizards are rupicolous and, at Augrabies, favour the granitic banks of the Orange River. The Augrabies population is extremely dense, resulting in a high encounter rate and frequent interactions between individuals. Detailed descriptions of the study area can be found in Branch & Whiting (1997) and Whiting & Greeff (1997).
(b) Chemical signal manipulation
We used hexane (Mason et al. 1989) to remove skin surface lipids from lizards. These individuals either served as scent donors or scentless controls or were labelled with extracted scent following scent removal (table 1). We randomly allocated females to one of three groups: control female (CF) in which scent was unmanipulated; hexane control female (HF) in which scent was removed; and hexane female with she-male scent (HFM) in which a female's scent was removed with hexane before being labelled with a she-male's scent. She-males were randomly allocated to the following groups: control she-male (CSM) in which scent was unmanipulated; hexane she-male (HSM) in which scent was removed; and hexane she-male with female scent (HSMF) in which a male's scent was removed with hexane before being labelled with a female's scent. We did not include he-males as a treatment group because the response is predictable: residents immediately challenge rivals and chase them (Whiting 1999; Whiting et al. 2003, 2006). Furthermore, there would be no chemical sampling in this scenario (M. J. Whiting 1995, personal observation) and, as such, the trials would not be informative. Lizards (table 1) were then presented once to a free-ranging male using tethering. We tethered the treatment male to a 3 m pole using an approximate 1 m length of dental floss, tied loosely around its waist. For consistency, the same individuals always either prepared lizard treatments or conducted the tethering trials, but not both. All trials were blind such that the presenter never knew which treatment he was presenting. To maintain independence we tested free-ranging males only once, over a broad area, and never worked in the same area twice. Free-ranging males showed the following reactions to the test animals: (i) continued courtship, (ii) aggression, or (iii) forced copulation attempts. We immediately terminated a trial if males were aggressive or attempted copulation.
Table 1.
Breakdown of treatments and sample sizes used to test chemical discrimination by free-ranging P. broadleyi males. (All individuals used in the treatments were female, or female-like in appearance (she-males). Individuals were allocated either to an unmanipulated control group, a control group in which hexane was used to remove all scent, or a treatment group in which scent was removed using hexane and lizards were relabelled male or female. All test lizards were used only once, and free-ranging he-males (N=208) were only presented with a single test lizard.)
treatment | description | N | gender | male scent | female scent | no scent |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
CF | control female | 35 | F | X | ||
CSM | control she-male | 34 | M | X | ||
HF | hexane female | 35 | F | X | ||
HSM | hexane she-male | 34 | M | X | ||
HFM | hexane female with male scent | 34 | F | X | ||
HSMF | hexane she-male with female scent | 36 | M | X |
(c) Visual signals and behavioural responses to chemical signals
We lowered the test animal several metres from the focal animal, in a visible location. For each trial, we recorded the following information: whether the focal animal approached using courtship; time to and time of first tongue-flick (TF); total number of tongue-flicks; whether the focal animal continued courtship following tongue-flicking; interaction time since first tongue-flick; total interaction time; and whether the focal animal attempted copulation and/or any aggressive behaviours. Duration of behaviours was recorded using stopwatches.
(d) Statistical analyses
Prior to conducting parametric procedures, we checked for normality using Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests and tested for homoscedacity using Bartlett's tests. Where necessary, we log-transformed data to meet the assumptions of parametric statistics. To assess scent recognition by males, we scored whether courting males continued with courtship after tongue-flicking the test animal. We used logistic regression in which an outcome is binary (court or not) to calculate the probability of a male courting a test animal in relation to a reference group (CFs). These odds ratios are calculated by taking the exponential of the coefficients of the model. Finally, we used logistic regression to test for significant differences in the probability of continued courtship among treatments.
3. Results
(a) Visual signals
Free-ranging males readily courted test lizards from all treatment groups (figure 2; logistic regression: Wald Χ52=5.5, p=0.36), suggesting that she-males are able to deceive males using visual signals. Furthermore, the odds of a male courting an individual from any of the treatment groups were similar to the odds of courting a CF (table 2; logistic regression: p>0.05 in each case). In particular, test males were 0.46 times as likely to court a CF compared with a CSM based on visual signals alone (Wald Χ12=0.38, p=0.54).
Figure 2.
The percentage of individuals courted by free-ranging males based on visual signals alone (prior to chemical sampling). Total sample sizes including both courted and non-courted individuals are given in table 1.
Table 2.
(a) Maximum-likelihood estimates testing for significant differences among treatment groups in the probability of courtship based purely on visual signals (prior to chemical sampling), in relation to the reference group (CFs). (The probability of a he-male courting a test lizard was independent of treatment.) (b) Odds ratio estimates of a he-male courting a lizard belonging to a treatment group compared with CFs based only on visual signals.
parameter | d.f. | estimate | s.e. | Wald Χ2 | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(a) | |||||
intercept | 1 | 3.219 | 1.020 | 9.963 | 0.002 |
CSM | 1 | −0.777 | 1.258 | 0.381 | 0.537 |
HF | 1 | −1.661 | 1.159 | 2.054 | 0.152 |
HFM | 1 | −1.347 | 1.153 | 1.366 | 0.243 |
HSM | 1 | −2.079 | 1.098 | 3.588 | 0.058 |
HSMF | 1 | −1.273 | 1.151 | 1.222 | 0.269 |
effect | point estimate | 95% Wald confidence limit | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(b) | |||||
CSM vs CF | 0.460 | 0.039–5.419 | |||
HF vs CF | 0.190 | 0.020–1.841 | |||
HFM vs CF | 0.260 | 0.027–2.489 | |||
HSM vs CF | 0.125 | 0.015–1.075 | |||
HSMF vs CF | 0.280 | 0.029–2.674 |
(b) Chemical signals
Scent treatment group had a significant effect on whether males continued courting test lizards after chemical sampling (logistic regression: Wald Χ52=16.82, p=0.005). Males based courtship on the presence of female scent irrespective of the true gender of the test lizard such that treatments with female scent had the highest frequency of courtship and treatments with male scent or no scent the lowest frequency of courtship (figure 3; table 3).
Figure 3.
The percentage of test individuals that were courted by free-ranging males following chemical sampling via tongue-flicking. Only individuals that approached the test animal using courtship are included here. Sample sizes are indicated above the bars.
Table 3.
(a) Maximum-likelihood estimates testing for significant differences among treatment groups in the probability of continued courtship after tongue-flicking, in relation to the reference group (CFs). (He-males were significantly more likely to court CFs than any treatment with male scent (CSM and HFM) and any treatment that lacked scent (HF and HSM); but he-males were not more likely to court she-males labelled with male scent (HSMF) compared with CFs.) (b) Odds ratio estimates of a he-male courting a lizard belonging to a treatment group compared with CFs based on chemical signals.
parameter | d.f. | estimate | s.e. | Wald Χ2 | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(a) | |||||
intercept | 1 | 1.335 | 0.503 | 7.055 | 0.008 |
CSM | 1 | −2.162 | 0.677 | 10.203 | 0.001 |
HF | 1 | −1.440 | 0.681 | 4.474 | 0.034 |
HFM | 1 | −1.971 | 0.650 | 9.193 | 0.002 |
HSM | 1 | −2.082 | 0.645 | 10.413 | 0.001 |
HSMF | 1 | −0.900 | 0.634 | 2.012 | 0.156 |
effect | point estimate | 95% Wald confidence limit | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(b) | |||||
CSM vs CF | 0.115 | 0.031–0.434 | |||
HF vs CF | 0.237 | 0.062–0.900 | |||
HFM vs CF | 0.139 | 0.039–0.498 | |||
HSM vs CF | 0.125 | 0.035–0.442 | |||
HSMF vs CF | 0.407 | 0.117–1.410 |
Following chemical sampling (first tongue-flick), interaction time among treatments was not significantly different (ANOVA, F5,201=1.86, p=0.1; see fig. S1 in the electronic supplementary material). However, if we group treatments according to whether they had male or female scent and include controls according to their true gender (CF, HF, HSMF versus CSM, HSM, HFM), free-ranging males spent significantly (t205=205, p=0.012) more time interacting with the ‘female’ group.
Following a log transformation, the number of tongue-flicks directed at test animals was homoscedastic (Bartlett's Χ52=4.3, p=0.51), and all treatments were normally distributed (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, D>0.1, p>0.13). The mean number of tongue-flicks by each treatment group were not significantly different (ANOVA, F5,201=1.36, p=0.24; see fig. S2 in the electronic supplementary material).
(c) The consequences of deception
The frequency of forced copulation attempts was significantly higher in the group containing females or she-males with female scent (CF, HF and HSMF) compared with the group containing she-males, or females with male scent (CSM, HSM and HFM; figure 4; Χ12=12.46, p=0.0001).
Figure 4.
The percentage of test animals that experienced copulation attempts by free-ranging males. See text for statistical analysis. Sample sizes are indicated above the bars.
4. Discussion
Male Augrabies flat lizards can delay the onset of colour expression and thereby mimic females, even though they are sexually mature. This resemblance was sufficient to conceal their true gender, such that similar numbers of he-males courted all treatment groups, regardless of their gender, on the basis of visual signals alone. However, based on the responses of he-males to test lizards, our experiment demonstrated that she-males do not mimic female pheromones. Females labelled with male scent and CSMs were very rarely courted following tongue-flicking and were sometimes met with an aggressive response (18% of trials compared with 0% for CFs). Conversely, CFs and HSMFs were courted by he-males following tongue-flicking and forced copulation attempts were highest in these groups (and the HF group). Also, when approached by he-males, she-males initially moved away and avoided male tongue-flicks. This is consistent with our observations of free-ranging she-males: close contact with he-males appears to be avoided at all costs. Therefore, while she-males exploit visual signals as a dishonest signal, their scent is a honest signal of their gender.
Multiple signals are used by most animals because they either reinforce information (‘backup messages’) or convey slightly different information (‘multiple messages’), usually about the signaller's condition, to a receiver (Johnstone 1996). Furthermore, different sensory modalities (e.g. acoustic, visual, chemical) are effective under specific environmental conditions (Johnstone 1997; Maynard Smith & Harper 2003; Hibbitts et al. 2007). Therefore, for animals to convey effective signals, multiple signal types might be employed depending on the environmental conditions at the time of signalling. Traditionally, most studies of multiple signalling have focused on mate choice (reviewed in Peschke 1987; Candolin 2003) or contest competition (Stuart-Fox et al. 2006; Whiting et al. 2006). In the context of female mimicry, the focus has been on single traits (but see Shine & Mason 2001); therefore, we have a very poor understanding of how receivers deal with multiple signals. While deception of visual signals such as pattern, colour and behaviour are relatively common (Brower 1988), mimicry of pheromonal signals, although best known for insects (Peschke 1987; Forsyth & Alcock 1990; Dettner & Liepert 1994; Cardé & Bel 1995), is extremely rare among vertebrates. In vertebrates, the only documented case of pheromonal mimicry in the context of female mimicry is the garter snake T. s. parietalis (Mason & Crews 1985). Interestingly, this is no longer considered an alternative reproductive tactic because female mimicry is a transitory phase adopted by probably all males in the first few days following emergence from hibernation (Shine et al. 2000). Among invertebrates, males of two species of rove beetle mimic female pheromones to avoid aggression from larger males (Peschke 1987; Forsyth & Alcock 1990). The nature of chemical mimicry precludes it from being a common phenomenon (Alberts 1992). Typically, a chemical signal is composed of numerous different components of variable molecular weight, aromaticity and volatility. Furthermore, chemical mimicry is far less likely when the biochemistry of odour production is tightly linked to nutritional status (Alberts 1992). In sexually selected species in which males are colourful, expression of colour is a natural ontogenetic process. Therefore, halting or reversing the expression of a normal physiological process may be a simpler process and therefore a lot more common. For example, depending on the prevailing social conditions, males of the cichlid Astatotilapia burtoni are able to rapidly switch between a non-territorial morph that mimics females and a colourful territorial morph (Korzan et al. 2008).
While she-male flat lizards are only able to deceive rival he-males with visual and not chemical signals, this may still be adaptive simply because the deception can be maintained through behavioural means. When in close quarters, she-males can avoid being chemically sampled by a short rapid movement to just beyond the reach of a male's tongue-flick. Many females move short distances away from courting males, so this behaviour is not unlike a female shunning the advances of a courting male. Finally, for she-males to successfully copulate with females, they may still require male scent as a necessary precursor during female mate choice. If this is true, then the fitness consequences of retaining male scent may override any benefits of mimicking female scent, particularly if the she-males are able to prevent males from chemically sampling them in the first place.
What are the benefits of female mimicry to she-males? Young males that are just developing adult male coloration on readily visible areas of the body (head and flanks) are frequently chased or attacked by older males that have more fighting experience and are typically larger. In Augrabies flat lizards, the best fighters have throats that are rich in UV while poor fighters have more violet throats (Whiting et al. 2006). She-males have little UV and almost never engage in contest competition (M. J. Whiting, J. K. Webb & J. S. Keogh 2003, unpublished data). By suppressing colour expression, males are able to mimic females and avoid the obvious risks associated with fighting that include injury, energetic costs and, possibly, predation. All males ultimately develop typical adult male coloration. Therefore, being a she-male is a transitory phase. The durations of these transitory phases are frequently condition-dependent strategies, which also occur in such diverse taxa as beetles (Peschke 1987; Forsyth & Alcock 1990), cuttlefish (Hanlon et al. 2005), cichlid fish (Korzan et al. 2008), garter snakes (Shine et al. 2000) and lizards (Martin & Forsman 1999). Although she-male flat lizards mimic females using visual and not chemical signals, by staying one step ahead of males and avoiding chemical contact, they are able to successfully maintain this deception. Consequently, she-males are able to approach, tongue-flick and court females in the presence of a resident male (M. J. Whiting, J. K. Webb & J. S. Keogh 2003, personal observation). Therefore, female mimicry in flat lizards is probably adaptive and favoured by sexual selection through its role as an alternative reproductive tactic.
Acknowledgments
We thank South African National Parks for permission to work at Augrabies Falls National Park (project no. 2000-11-08MWHI), and for the support of various personnel at Augrabies. M.J.W. was supported by grants from the National Research Foundation and the University of the Witwatersrand, and J.K.W. and J.S.K. thank the Australian Research Council for its ongoing support. We also thank Joanne Keogh for thoroughly scrubbing lizard boxes in the field, Bob Mason for advice on using hexane to remove lizard pheromones and skin lipids, and Alecia Kirton for excellent statistical advice. This work was cleared by the University of the Witwatersrand Animal Ethics Committee (2003-80-3).
Supplementary Material
Figure S2. Mean (±1 s.e.) number of tongue-flicks directed at test lizards by free-raging males for untransformed data
References
- Adams E.S., Caldwell R.L. Deceptive communication in asymmetric fights of the stomatopod crustacean Gonodactylus bredini. Anim. Behav. 1990;39:706–716. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80382-3 [Google Scholar]
- Alberts A.C. Constraints on the design of chemical communication systems in terrestrial vertebrates. Am. Nat. 1992;139:S62–S89. doi:10.1086/285305 [Google Scholar]
- Andersson M. Princeton University Press; Princeton, NJ: 1994. Sexual selection. [Google Scholar]
- Andersson M., Iwasa Y. Sexual selection [Review] Trends Ecol. Evol. 1996;11:A53–A58. doi: 10.1016/0169-5347(96)81042-1. doi:10.1016/0169-5347(96)81042-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson S., Pryke S.R., Ornborg J., Lawes M.J., Andersson M. Multiple receivers, multiple ornaments, and a trade-off between agonistic and epigamic signaling in a widowbird. Am. Nat. 2002;160:683–691. doi: 10.1086/342817. doi:10.1086/342817 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Backwell P.R.Y., Christy J.H., Telford S.R., Jennions M.D., Passmore N.I. Dishonest signalling in a fiddler crab. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2000;267:719–724. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1062. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Branch W.R., Whiting M.J. A new Platysaurus (Squamata: Cordylidae) from the Northern Cape Province, South Africa. Afr. J. Herpetol. 1997;46:124–136. [Google Scholar]
- Brandt Y. Lizard threat display handicaps endurance. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2003;270:1061–1068. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2343. doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2343 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briffa M. Signal residuals during shell fighting in hermit crabs: can costly signals be used deceptively? Behav. Ecol. 2006;17:510–514. doi:10.1093/beheco/arj059 [Google Scholar]
- Brower L.P. The University of Chicago Press; Chicago, IL: 1988. Mimicry and the evolutionary process. [Google Scholar]
- Burley N. The evolution of sexual indistinguishability. In: Alexander R.D., Tinkle D.W., editors. Natural selection and social behavior. Chiron; New York, NY: 1982. pp. 121–137. [Google Scholar]
- Candolin U. The use of multiple cues in mate choice. Biol. Rev. 2003;78:575–595. doi: 10.1017/s1464793103006158. doi:10.1017/S1464793103006158 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardé R.T., Bel W.J. Springer; Berlin, Germany: 1995. Chemical ecology of insects 2. [Google Scholar]
- Clutton-Brock T.H., Albon S.D., Gibson R.M., Guinness F.E. The logical stag: adaptive aspects of fighting in red deer (Cervus elaphus L.) Anim. Behav. 1979;27:211–225. [Google Scholar]
- Cooper, W. E. & Greenberg, N. 1992 Reptilian coloration and behavior. In Biology of the reptilia, vol. 18 (eds C. Gans & D. Crews), pp. 298–422. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.
- Cooper W.E., Vitt L.J. Deferred agonistic behavior in a long-lived scincid lizard Eumeces laticeps: field and laboratory data on the roles of body size and residence in agonistic strategy. Oecologia. 1987;72:321–326. doi: 10.1007/BF00377558. doi:10.1007/BF00377558 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dettner K., Liepert C. Chemical mimicry and camouflage. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 1994;39:129–154. doi:10.1146/annurev.en.39.010194.001021 [Google Scholar]
- Dominey W.J. Female mimicry in male bluegill sunfish—a genetic polymorphism? Nature. 1980;284:546–548. doi:10.1038/284546a0 [Google Scholar]
- Endler J.A. Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of evolution. Am. Nat. 1992;139:S125–S153. doi:10.1086/285308 [Google Scholar]
- Endler J.A. The color of light in forests and its implications. Ecol. Monogr. 1993;36:1–27. doi:10.2307/2937121 [Google Scholar]
- Forsyth A., Alcock J. Female mimicry and resource defense polygyny by males of a tropical rove beetle, Leistotrophus versicolor (Coleoptera, Staphylinidae) Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1990;26:325–330. doi:10.1007/BF00171097 [Google Scholar]
- Gross M.R. Alternative reproductive strategies and tactics: diversity within sexes. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1996;11:92–97. doi: 10.1016/0169-5347(96)81050-0. doi:10.1016/0169-5347(96)81050-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanlon R.T., Naud M.-J., Shaw P.W., Havenhand J.N. Transient sexual mimicry leads to fertilisation. Nature. 2005;430:212. doi: 10.1038/433212a. doi:10.1038/433212a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hews D.K., Benard M.F. Negative association between conspicuous visual display and chemosensory behavior in two phrynosomatid lizards. Ethology. 2001;107:839–850. doi:10.1046/j.1439-0310.2001.00712.x [Google Scholar]
- Hibbitts T.J., Whiting M.J., Stuart-Fox D.M. Shouting the odds: vocalization signals status in a lizard. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2007;61:1169–1176. doi:10.1007/s00265-006-0330-x [Google Scholar]
- Hughes M. The function of concurrent signals: visual and chemical communication in snapping shrimp. Anim. Behav. 1996;52:247–257. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0170 [Google Scholar]
- Hughes M. Deception with honest signals: signal residuals and signal function in snapping shrimp. Behav. Ecol. 2000;11:614–623. doi:10.1093/beheco/11.6.614 [Google Scholar]
- Johnstone R.A. Honest advertisement of multiple qualities using multiple signals. J. Theor. Biol. 1995a;177:87–94. doi:10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80006-2 [Google Scholar]
- Johnstone R.A. Sexual selection, honest advertisement and the handicap principle: reviewing the evidence. Biol. Rev. 1995b;70:1–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185x.1995.tb01439.x. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.1995.tb01439.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnstone R.A. Multiple displays in animal communication: ‘backup signals’ and ‘multiple messages’. Proc. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. 1996;351:329–338. doi:10.1098/rstb.1996.0026 [Google Scholar]
- Johnstone R.A. The evolution of animal signals. In: Krebs J.R., Davies N.B., editors. Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Blackwell; Oxford, UK: 1997. pp. 155–178. [Google Scholar]
- Korzan W.J., Robison R.R., Zhao S., Fernald R.D. Color change as a potential behavioral strategy. Horm. Behav. 2008;54:463–470. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.05.006. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.05.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopez P., Martin J. Pheromonal recognition of females takes precedence over the chromatic cue in male Iberian wall lizards Podarcis hispanica. Ethology. 2001;107:901–912. doi:10.1046/j.1439-0310.2001.00724.x [Google Scholar]
- Lopez P., Martin J., Cuadrado M. Pheromone-mediated intrasexual aggression in male lizards Podarcis hispanicus. Aggress. Behav. 2002;28:154–163. doi:10.1002/ab.90017 [Google Scholar]
- Martin J., Forsman A. Social costs and development of nuptial coloration in male Psammodromus algirus lizards: an experiment. Behav. Ecol. 1999;10:396–400. doi:10.1093/beheco/10.4.396 [Google Scholar]
- Mason R.T., Crews D. Female mimicry in garter snakes. Nature. 1985;316:59–60. doi: 10.1038/316059a0. doi:10.1038/316059a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mason R.T., Fales H.M., Jones T.H., Pannell L.K., Chinn J.W., Crews D. Sex pheromones in snakes. Science. 1989;245:290–293. doi: 10.1126/science.2749261. doi:10.1126/science.2749261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maynard Smith J., Harper D. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: 2003. Animal signals. [Google Scholar]
- Møller A.P. Deceptive use of alarm calls by male swallows, Hirundo rustica: a new paternity guard. Behav. Ecol. 1990;1:1–6. doi:10.1093/beheco/1.1.1 [Google Scholar]
- Peschke K. Male aggression, female mimicry and female choice in the rove beetle, Aleochara curtula (Coleoptera, Staphylinidae) Ethology. 1987;75:265–284. [Google Scholar]
- Rowe C. Receiver psychology and the evolution of multicomponent signals. Anim. Behav. 1999;58:921–931. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1242. doi:10.1006/anbe.1999.1242 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saetre G.P., Slagsvold T. The significance of female mimicry in male contests. Am. Nat. 1996;147:981–995. doi:10.1086/285889 [Google Scholar]
- Shine R., Mason R.T. Courting male garter snakes (Thamnophis sirtalis parietalis) use multiple cues to identify potential mates. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2001;49:465–473. doi:10.1007/s002650100334 [Google Scholar]
- Shine R., Harlow P., Lemaster M.P., Moore I.T., Mason R.T. The transvestite serpent: why do male garter snakes court (some) other males? Anim. Behav. 2000;59:349–359. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1321. doi:10.1006/anbe.1999.1321 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shine R., Phillips B., Waye H., LeMaster M.P., Mason R.T. Benefits of female mimicry in snakes. Nature. 2001;414:267. doi: 10.1038/35104687. doi:10.1038/35104687 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinervo B., Lively C. The rock-paper-scissors game and the evolution of alternative male strategies. Nature. 1996;380:240–243. doi:10.1038/380240a0 [Google Scholar]
- Slagsvold T., Saetre G.-P. Evolution of plumage color in male pied flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca): evidence for female mimicry. Evolution. 1991;45:910–917. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1991.tb04359.x. doi:10.2307/2409698 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stapley J., Whiting M.J. Ultraviolet signals fighting ability in a lizard. Biol. Lett. 2006;2:169–172. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2005.0419. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2005.0419 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuart-Fox D.M., Firth D., Moussalli A., Whiting M.J. Multiple signals in chameleon contests: designing and analysing animal contests as a tournament. Anim. Behav. 2006;71:1263–1271. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.07.028 [Google Scholar]
- Whiting M.J. When to be neighbourly: differential agonistic responses in the lizard Platysaurus broadleyi. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1999;46:210–214. doi:10.1007/s002650050611 [Google Scholar]
- Whiting M.J., Bateman P.W. Male preference for large females in the lizard Platysaurus broadleyi. J. Herpetol. 1999;33:309–312. doi:10.2307/1565730 [Google Scholar]
- Whiting M.J., Greeff J.M. Facultative frugivory in the Cape flat lizard, Platysaurus capensis (Savria: Cordylidae) Copeia. 1997;1997:811–818. doi:10.2307/1447298 [Google Scholar]
- Whiting M.J., Nagy K.A., Bateman P.W. Evolution and maintenance of social status signalling badges: experimental manipulations in lizards. In: Fox S.F., McCoy J.K., Baird T.A., editors. Lizard social behavior. Johns Hopkins University Press; Baltimore, MD: 2003. pp. 47–82. [Google Scholar]
- Whiting M.J., Stuart-Fox D.M., O'Connor D., Firth D., Bennett N.C., Blomberg S.P. Ultraviolet signals ultra-aggression in a lizard. Anim. Behav. 2006;72:353–363. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.10.018 [Google Scholar]
- Wikelski M., Carbone C., Trillmich F. Lekking in marine iguanas: female grouping and male reproductive strategies. Anim. Behav. 1996;52:581–596. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0199 [Google Scholar]
- Zahavi A., Zahavi A. Oxford University Press; New York, NY: 1997. The handicap principle. [Google Scholar]
- Zamudio K.R., Sinervo E. Polygyny, mate-guarding, and posthumous fertilization as alternative male mating strategies. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2000;97:14 427–14 432. doi: 10.1073/pnas.011544998. doi:10.1073/pnas.011544998 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Figure S2. Mean (±1 s.e.) number of tongue-flicks directed at test lizards by free-raging males for untransformed data