Skip to main content
Royal Society Open Science logoLink to Royal Society Open Science
. 2021 Jul 7;8(7):210266. doi: 10.1098/rsos.210266

Social dominance and cooperation in female vampire bats

Rachel J Crisp 1,2,, Lauren J N Brent 1, Gerald G Carter 2,3,†,
PMCID: PMC8261227  PMID: 34295524

Abstract

When group-living animals develop individualized social relationships, they often regulate cooperation and conflict through a dominance hierarchy. Female common vampire bats have been an experimental system for studying cooperative relationships, yet surprisingly little is known about female conflict. Here, we recorded the outcomes of 1023 competitive interactions over food provided ad libitum in a captive colony of 33 vampire bats (24 adult females and their young). We found a weakly linear dominance hierarchy using three common metrics (Landau's h’ measure of linearity, triangle transitivity and directional consistency). However, patterns of female dominance were less structured than in many other group-living mammals. Female social rank was not clearly predicted by body size, age, nor reproductive status, and competitive interactions were not correlated with kinship, grooming nor food sharing. We therefore found no evidence that females groomed or shared food up a hierarchy or that differences in rank explained asymmetries in grooming or food sharing. A possible explanation for such apparently egalitarian relationships among female vampire bats is the scale of competition. Female vampire bats that are frequent roostmates might not often directly compete for food in the wild.

Keywords: competition, conflict, dominance hierarchy, interdependence, social rank

1. Introduction

Early studies of social dominance and intra-sexual competition often focused on males due in part to their greater reproductive skew and the relative ease of observing male–male competition, but female social dominance is now known to play a key role in structuring many animal societies [1,2]. Female social dominance is most obvious in cooperatively breeding species in which dominant females can monopolize breeding by suppressing reproduction of subordinate females, and intense female competition selects for traits that are more typically male, such as large body size and bright colours [24]. Female social rank also shapes social structure in species where all females can breed. In many primates, where social ranks and social structures of natural populations are often well documented, reproductive success is influenced by a female's network of individualized social relationships, each involving a mix of cooperation and conflict [510]. Long-term field studies of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) and baboons (Papio sp.) show that females form highly stable and strongly linear dominance hierarchies with clear hierarchical relationships between individuals and between matrilines [11,12], and that female social rank predicts female fitness [8,1315]. Rank also predicts which individuals form social bonds; baboon and rhesus macaque females typically form bonds with those of a similar rank to themselves [1621].

Many primates regulate their social relationships by social grooming, which could be related to social rank in several ways. For example, in groups with steep hierarchies, low-ranked individuals often direct grooming up the hierarchy, whereas in groups with shallow dominance hierarchies, grooming is often more symmetrical [1629]. It remains unclear whether such patterns apply to non-primates that also form complex individualized cooperative relationships (but see [30]). The goal of this study is to assess (i) if female vampire bats have clear dominance ranks and (ii) if rank influences social grooming or food sharing.

Female common vampire bats (Desmodus rotundus) have been used to study the functions and development of cooperative relationships [31], but surprisingly little is known about female conflict or social rank in this species. Common vampire bats seem to share many convergent life history and social traits with primates despite their lineages diverging about 60–70 Ma [32]. When compared with other closely related bats, vampire bats have an extended longevity [33], prolonged period of offspring dependency [3436], exceptional rates of social grooming [3739], and high levels of social complexity, involving the formation of individualized social relationships [31,4042].

Vampire bats live in highly fluid societies, in which compositions of roosting groups change frequently, and individuals interact to varying degrees with kin and many non-kin. In Costa Rica, colonies included several groups of 8–12 adult females and their offspring within hollow trees [43,44], but in other sites larger aggregations occur that might include cryptic subgroups [42,45]. Although female vampire bats are largely philopatric and form multiple matrilines, most females are not close kin, maternal siblings are unlikely to share fathers, and an adult female might immigrate into a group about every 2 years. As a consequence, kinship within roosts is on average low (0.07–0.08) but highly variable [42,46,47] similar to other mammals with high relationship complexity [9]. For example, the same average kinship is seen in female yellow baboons, eastern gorillas, and Asian elephants [9].

Male vampire bats have clear dominance relationships with other males that determine access to territories in roosts, such as hollows in trees or caves, that attract females [46]. A dominant male guards a territory and can father almost half the offspring of females that occupy their roosts during their tenure of up to 17 months; however, they do not appear to control individual females, which are larger than males [45,46]. Young male vampire bats disperse at 12–18 months of age, possibly in response to aggression from the resident dominant male [44,46].

Despite frequent movements between and within roosts, females tend to roost near preferred individuals that are also grooming and food-sharing partners [4244]. Regurgitated food sharing appears to be critical for vampire bats because they regularly fail to feed in the wild and have a poor capacity to store energy [43,48]. Food donation rates are predicted by grooming, reciprocal donations and kinship [40], and new food-sharing bonds develop through escalations of reciprocal grooming [31]. Vampire bats groom each other more than other bat species and social grooming is more frequent in females than males [37,38].

Little is known about female social dominance. Agonistic interactions during feeding at wound sites suggests that dominance relationships could determine access to food. Vampire bats sometimes fight over wound sites in the wild (or feeders in captivity) and they will aggressively defend, or take over, a wound site from others [44,45,4953]. In one small captive study, a female was dominant over two males [50], and in another captive group of six females and three male vampire bats, Park [52] observed pushing and fighting, and submissive behaviours, such as waiting for feeding bats to leave before approaching to feed. Young bats engaged in aggressive behaviours more than adults, dominant males were submissive to females and the most dominant individual fed first, but other bats did not follow a clear feeding order [52]. These observations suggest a dominance hierarchy both between and within sexes, especially in males, but no study to date has rigorously measured dominance structure or tested if social rank influences cooperative behaviours in female vampire bats.

Here, we recorded the outcomes of competitive interactions over food among a captive colony of 33 vampire bats (24 adult females and their non-reproductive offspring). We then assessed the existence and steepness of a dominance hierarchy using Landau's h’ measure of linearity, triangle transitivity and directional consistency. For context, we then compared these measures of female dominance with those of other species. We then tested (i) if social rank was predicted by body size, age or reproductive status, (ii) if kinship, grooming rates or food-sharing rates predicted female competitive interactions, (iii) if grooming or food sharing was directed up the hierarchy, and (iv) if asymmetries in grooming or food sharing could be explained by differences in female social rank.

2. Material and methods

2.1. Study subjects and animal care

Subjects were a captive colony of 33 common vampire bats (Desmodus rotundus), housed in a 2.3 × 4.5 × 2.5 m flight cage at the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute in Gamboa, Panamá for a 2-year experiment on the development of cooperative relationships in female vampire bats [31]. The colony included 24 adult females captured in Panamá from two distant sites: Las Pavas (n = 7) and Tolé (n = 17), as well as nine young bats (four males, five females) that were born in captivity between 3 June 2016 and 15 December 2016. We sampled competitive interactions from 1 November 2016 to 31 January 2017. Bats were individually marked using a unique combination of bands of four types (coloured, round, shiny, dull) on their forearms. Bats were able to feed from a row of 3–10 spouts of blood on the floor of the cage between the hours of 18.00 and 9.00. To prevent coagulation, we added 11 g of sodium citrate and 4 g of citric acid per 4 l of bovine or porcine blood collected from a slaughterhouse.

2.2. Kinship and cooperative relationships

We used previously published kinship estimates that were based on known maternities and 17 microsatellite markers [42]. We also used previously published rates of dyadic grooming and food sharing from a series of fasting trials, where a focal bat was isolated and fasted alone and then introduced to the group for one hour to induce food sharing and grooming [31]. For all analyses, behaviour rates were transformed using natural log (x + 1), where x is the seconds of interaction per fasting trial divided by the number of trials where grooming or sharing food was possible [31]. We only used grooming and food-sharing data from the start of the colony until 31 January 2017, when we finished sampling competitive interactions, but results did not change if we instead used all observations of grooming and sharing.

2.3. Competitive interactions

To observe competitive interactions, we video-recorded feeders using an infrared-illuminated surveillance camera from 17.30 to 8.30 on 70 nights (1050 h). We identified ‘winners’ and ‘losers’ from five types of events at the feeders:

  • 1.

    Contact intrude: a feeding bat is replaced at the feeder by an intruding bat using physical contact (e.g. electronic supplementary material, video S1). The intruder is the winner.

  • 2.

    No-contact intrude: the same as ‘contact intrude’ but without physical contact.

  • 3.

    Contact defend: a feeding bat uses physical contact to maintain its position at the feeder following an approach by another bat. The defender is the winner.

  • 4.

    No-contact defend: the same as ‘contact defend’ but without physical contact.

  • 5.

    Waiting: A bat in view does not begin to feed until a feeding bat leaves the feeder. The waiting bat is the loser.

From these events, we created win–lose matrices, which summed the total number of wins made by each individual against every other. We also created a win–lose matrix which combined all event types. To ensure we had a sufficient sample of observations per individual to infer ranks in a moderately steep dominance hierarchy, we followed guidelines recommended by Sánchez-Tójar et al. [54] that: (i) all bats interacted with another bat at least once, (ii) there were on average 10 sampled interaction events per bat, a sampling effort recommended for detecting a moderately steep dominance hierarchy, and (iii) the proportion of dyads that we observed interacting (66%), was greater than what is expected from a Poisson-based null model (46%, 95% CI: 36–58%).

2.4. Measuring group-level dominance

For comparison with other species, we assessed dominance structure at the group level using six previously established measures, using the ‘compete’ R package [55]. We quantified linearity via two common metrics: de Vries h’ index [56] and triangle transitivity [57]. Linearity and transitivity mean that for any given triad within the group: if A dominates B, and B dominates C, then C should not dominate A or B. Triangle transitivity can lead to more accurate estimations of linearity than Landau's h’ when observations are sparse or when many dyads have unknown interaction types. Dominance interactions within each dyad are also expected to be asymmetric, which we quantified via directional consistency [58], which measures the mean frequency that a behaviour is performed in one direction, relative to the total number of times it is performed in either direction. To test for group-level dominance, we compared each of these observed metrics with the distribution (and 95% quantiles) of metrics expected under the null hypothesis, which we generated by randomizing the direction of dyadic event data 5000 times. We also fit a linear model to test how each observed metric changed with an increasing number of observations drawn without replacement in random order.

Dominance hierarchies can vary in steepness and shape. ‘Despotic’ hierarchies are characterized by highly linear dominance relationships and access to resources being heavily skewed towards a small proportion of dominant individuals. In ‘egalitarian’ hierarchies, weakly linear dominance relationships lead to lower skew in resource access [59]. These differences can be quantified by hierarchy steepness, which we measured as the slope of a line fitted to the relationship between individual ranks and normalized David's scores [60]. We also assessed hierarchy shape using a plot of how well rank differences predicted the propensity to win [54]. In addition to allowing comparisons between datasets, plotting confidence intervals on the winning probability of dominant individuals provides insight into the certainty that an estimated hierarchy reflects a real underlying dominance structure.

2.5. Comparisons with other species

To put our results in context, we compared group-level metrics with 14 other datasets of female social rank in mammals, and with 172 dominance interaction matrices from 84 different species ranging from invertebrates to birds to mammals, compiled by Shizuka & McDonald [57]. To assess certainty of our dominance hierarchy, we also compared measures of ‘uncertainty by repeatability’ and ‘uncertainty by splitting’ [54] to those in the 172 other datasets.

2.6. Assigning social rank to individuals

We assigned social ranks to individual bats using three measures: David's score, Elo-rating and Glicko-rating. David's score calculates individual ranks by summing wins and losses for each individual scaled to the summed scores of their interaction partners [61], a method which performs well in comparisons with others [6264], and can be estimated from the win–lose matrix. Elo-rating calculates individual rank using a common numerical starting score for all individuals that is updated with each competitive interaction to give a final rank [65,66]. Glicko-rating [67] differs from Elo-rating in that the points which are gained or lost following an interaction are not matched in both players, but are instead adjusted according to a function of the dyad's difference in rating and their respective rating deviations.

Because Elo-rating and Glicko-rating begin by allocating all individuals with an identical starting score from which ratings diverge as interactions accumulate, there is a ‘burn-in’ period during which ranks are unreliable until enough observations have been recorded to reflect the true rank order, and the length of this period can be impossible to determine without prior knowledge of a hierarchy's structure [67]. Indeed, if individuals interact infrequently or the hierarchy is not steep, then the burn-in period may exceed the duration of the study [67]. Thus, without stable ranks, prior knowledge of dominance relationships or a high frequency of interactions, it may be more effective or conservative to use matrix-based ranking methods like David's score [68].

2.7. Predictors of individual social rank

To examine whether rank was predicted by body size, we plotted and fit general linear models to test if rank varied with either mean body mass or forearm length (standard proxies for body size in bats). Forearm length has much greater repeatability in vampire bats given that their mass changes dramatically with feeding and urination. To assess whether rank correlated with other characteristics, we also tested for rank differences by age category (adult versus young), age within young bats, maternity (mothers versus non-mothers) and source population (Las Pavas versus Tolé).

2.8. Comparing competitive and cooperative interactions

To test if higher-ranking bats had higher mean rates of grooming or sharing food towards other bats, we used Pearson's r correlation. To test for correlations between networks, we used Mantel tests with Pearson's r using the vegan R package [69]. To test two simultaneous predictors on a response, we used multiple regression quadratic assignment procedure with double semi-partialling (MRQAP) in the asnipe R package [70]. To assess whether social rank influenced the time when bats were observed at feeders (e.g. if higher-ranked females fed earlier), we used a permutation test in which we compared the observed regression coefficient for the effect of an actor's rank on event time with the expected coefficient values when each actor was re-assigned a random rank throughout the entire dataset. We used 5000 permutations in all these permutation tests.

Since bats with preferred relationships might be more likely to feed at the same time and hence compete, we used MRQAP to test if conflict rates were predicted by networks of kinship, grooming or food sharing when controlling for number of dyadic observations in the same hour. Our findings did not differ when not controlling for number of dyadic observations. To test if bats tended to groom up or down the dominance hierarchy, we made a matrix of rank difference (receiver rank–actor rank), then used MRQAP to test if dyadic grooming given was predicted by both dyadic grooming received and rank difference. By including both effects, we could test whether rank differences could help explain asymmetries in dyadic grooming. We repeated this same test with food sharing instead of grooming. Results were the same when replacing all negative rank differences with zero to test the hypothesis that bats only directed help upwards and did not discriminate between lower-ranking individuals. To test if bats tended to groom or share food with females that were closer in rank, we also conducted the same tests using the absolute rank difference.

To test if helping (grooming or food sharing) was more symmetrical in dyads of closer rank, we constructed networks of helping symmetry defined as the difference between dyadic help that was given and received divided by the total of help given and help received. We then tested the correlation between this symmetry value and absolute rank difference. For each female, we assessed the total proportion of help that was directed up the hierarchy and tested if this was different to 50%. We also measured the mean difference in the interaction rate for partners that were higher rank versus lower rank and tested if this was different than zero. In both cases, we tested this by calculating a 95% confidence interval around the mean using bootstrapping (5000 iterations, percentile method in the boot package [71,72]).

3. Results

3.1. Dominance structure at the group level

Out of 1300 recorded events at feeders, there were 1023 cases where actor and receiver bats could be identified by their bands. Metrics of group-level dominance were largely similar across the different types of events (224 contact intruding, 250 no-contact intruding, 214 contact defending, 109 no-contact defending, 219 waiting and 7 ambiguous events, electronic supplementary material and figure S1). Therefore, we combined wins from all types of events to create a single win–lose matrix (electronic supplementary material, figure S2). We observed 31 interaction events per bat (62 wins or losses, median = 46, range = 15–162).

We detected clear evidence for female–female dominance structure using de Vries h’ (0.30, p < 0.0002), directional consistency (0.59, p < 0.0002) and triangle transitivity (0.90, p < 0.0002, figure 1) when using all event types and all bats. Directional consistency and triangle transitivity were also evident when using only the data within each event type (figure 1). Results were the same when including four young males (de Vries h’: 0.28, p < 0.0002), directional consistency (0.61, p < 0.0002) and triangle transitivity (0.89, p < 0.0002).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Group-level female dominance structure using three measures of dominance. Dots show observed metric and lines show the 95% confidence intervals of the metrics expected under the null hypothesis (when the direction of event data was randomized) for each event type (grey) and for all event types (black). Randomly sampling more observations led to higher de Vries h’ (R2 = 0.58, slope = 1.9, n = 1016, p < 0.0001) and triangle transitivity (R2 = 0.58, slope = 1.9, n = 1016, p < 0.0001) and lower directional consistency (R2 = 0.44, slope = 0.0001, p < 0.0001). See electronic supplementary material, figure S1 for comparison of observed dominance measures.

Group-level dominance measures in the female vampire bats (excluding four young males) were lower than in most other groups of female mammals (figure 2). Directional consistency, triangle transitivity, and Landau's h’ values were in the bottom 6%, 5% and 2%, respectively, for 172 comparison taxa [57]. Likewise, uncertainty by repeatability and by splitting [54] was greater than 95% of estimates from comparison taxa, which cannot be explained by sampling effort, indicating a shallow hierarchy with relatively low confidence in individual ranks.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Comparison of group-level dominance measures in female vampire bats relative to 15 other datasets of female mammal groups. Data from Shizuka & McDonald [57].

The female hierarchy steepness based on David's score was 0.19 (p < 0.0001) or 0.18 (p < 0.0001) when including four young males. The female hierarchy shape based on David's score showed that rank estimates did not clearly determine winning rates, especially when compared with species with a clear female dominance hierarchy (figure 3).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Hierarchy shape of female vampire bats compared with yellow baboons and long-tailed macaques. A greater difference in rank (David's score) should yield a higher probability that the higher rank wins. Error bars and shading are 95% CIs of points and the fit of local polynomial regression (ggplot2 R package). Dashed line shows probability expected by chance. Data from yellow baboons Papio cynocephalus are from Hausfater et al. [73]. Data from long-tailed macaques Macaca fascicularis are from Sterck & Steenbeek [74]. For comparison of hierarchy shape of female vampire bats by method, see electronic supplementary material, figure S3.

3.2. Individual ranks and predictors of rank

Individual ranks from David's score, Elo-rating and Glicko-rating were highly correlated (n = 33 bats including four young males, David's score versus Elo-rating: r = 0.76, p < 0.001; David's score versus Glicko-rating: r = 0.85, p < 0.001; Elo-rating versus Glicko-rating, r = 0.93, p < 0.001; electronic supplementary material, figure S4). We used David's score as ranks for subsequent analyses, because it can be replicated from the win–lose matrix, produced the clearest hierarchy shape (electronic supplementary material, figure S3), and was the most correlated overall with other methods, winning rate and losing rate. Alternate analyses with ranks based on the other two methods did not give different results, except where noted.

Young males had lower ranks on average than females (t = −2.47, n = 33, p = 0.019, figure 4). We detected evidence for an interaction effect between forearm size and age (t = 3.33, n = 33 bats, p = 0.002), because longer forearms were positively correlated with rank in the smaller young males and females (t = 3.04, n = 9, p = 0.02), but were negatively correlated with rank in the larger adult females (t = −2.69, n = 23, p = 0.01; electronic supplementary material, figure S3). The highest-ranking bats therefore tended to be intermediate in size (electronic supplementary material, figure S3). When excluding males, we detected only that females with smaller forearms tended to be more dominant (t = −2.78, n = 29, p = 0.01). Across all bats, we detected no difference in rank by body mass (t = −1.33, n = 33, p = 0.2), ‘body condition’ (mass divided by forearm, t = 0.56, n = 33, p = 0.6), source population (t = −0.09, n = 24, p = 0.9), being a mother (t = 1.8, n = 33, p = 0.08) or age of young bats (t = −0.7, n = 9, p = 0.5). We did not detect evidence that higher-ranking bats were observed at feeders earlier in the night than expected by chance (β = −0.24, n = 2313 observations, p = 0.11).

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Social rank by age and sex. Rank is based on David's score (higher number is higher rank). Boxplots show the median (thick line), the first and third quartiles (box).

We did not find that higher-ranking adult females performed more grooming (r = 0.15, n = 24, p = 0.5) or food sharing (r = −0.04, p = 0.9). Among the 24 adult females, we also failed to find that competitive interaction rates were predicted by kinship (β = −0.24, p = 0.7), grooming (β = −0.03, p = 0.8), or food sharing (β = −0.35, p = 0.2), and results remained the same whether or not we controlled for the overlap in time at the feeders. We also did not find evidence that grooming or sharing was directed up or down the dominance hierarchy (grooming: β = −0.05, p = 0.3, sharing: β = 0.07, p = 0.4, n = 24). Grooming was not detectably biased towards females of closer rank (β = 0.003, p = 0.9). Instead, we found unexpected weak evidence that females preferentially fed females of more distant rank in either direction (β = 0.18, p = 0.037); but this weak trend was not robust (i.e. not detected using other rank measures or after controlling for multiple p-values). Finally, we did not find evidence that rank differences correlated with the symmetry of grooming (r = −0.09, p = 0.9) or sharing (r = 0.03, p = 0.4; figure 5).

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

No evidence that females directed grooming or food sharing up a dominance hierarchy. Left panel shows the proportion of grooming or food sharing that each female (blue circle) directed to higher-ranking females. Triangles show means with bootstrapped 95% CI. Dashed line shows random expectation. Right panel shows the difference for each bat (line) in the mean interaction rates (circles) across female recipients that were either lower (down) or higher (up) in rank. Means with bootstrapped 95% CIs show that the overall within-bat difference is not different than zero. These plots are for visualization; actual inferences were based on more powerful permutation tests using rank differences rather than just direction.

4. Discussion

Our findings suggest that female common vampire bats form a dominance hierarchy that is weakly linear and shallow, suggesting egalitarian access to resources among familiar bats. Dominance structure among females in our study was more linear and transitive than expected from random interactions (figure 1), but also weaker relative to many other female mammals (figure 2). In this captive colony, the ranks of female vampire bats (with or without their young) were less stable, less linear, and less despotic than the majority (over 90%) of datasets from 172 other comparison species [57]. As expected with a shallow and weakly linear hierarchy, the outcomes of competitive interactions were only predictable when there was a large difference in ranks (figure 3).

Given a shallow hierarchy and inability to assign ranks with high confidence, it is not too surprising that we did not find evidence that ranks were explained using other measures. Rank was not predicted by body mass, sex, age, reproductive status or location of origin, except that young male bats had lower ranks than females (figure 4). Our findings suggest that the relationship between forearm size and rank might be nonlinear or depend on age (electronic supplementary material, figure S5), but more observations are necessary to confirm this idea. These findings are consistent with three non-mutually exclusive possibilities: female social rank might not be driven by physical traits, individual ranks might not be sufficiently precise to detect effects, and winning rates might be more influenced by other factors such as winner–loser effects [75].

Contrary to patterns in some female primates [23,24], we found no evidence that females directed grooming or food sharing to higher-ranking females, or that rates of grooming, food sharing or their symmetry, were biased to closer ranked females. We also found no clear evidence that grooming or food sharing is exchanged for tolerance from higher-ranked females (e.g. [76]), which is consistent with other evidence that grooming and food sharing promotes reciprocal investments, rather than mere tolerance [31,39,40,43]. However, a lack of a clear relationship between cooperative relationships (based on food-sharing and grooming) and rank (based on interactions at feeders) is not clear evidence for no relationship between these behaviours. The findings of our study must be interpreted with several important limitations in mind.

First, like many other studies based on visual observations, many dyads were observed interacting only a few times or were never observed to interact, so the precision of winning rates varies and many dyadic winning rates are likely to be imprecise (electronic supplementary material, figure S1). One could address this issue by forcing bats to compete over limited food in pairwise interactions.

Second, displacements and waiting at feeders might not convey dominance. If grooming is exchanged for tolerance during feeding, then this effect may negate the ability to detect dominance, because more dominant females would allow subordinates who groom them to also feed near them or even displace them from the blood spout, especially if spouts are not limited. Similarly, displacement rates could also be a poor proxy for actual dominance relationships if higher-ranking individuals make space for subordinate individuals for non-competitive reasons. For example, a dominant individual feeding alone might prefer to have a subordinate individual beside them to dilute predation risk. One could potentially address these issues by making food more limited when observing dominance, by scoring dominance in more explicitly aggressive conflicts, or forcing bats to compete for limited spatial positions in another way (e.g. positions closer to a heat source in a cold environment).

Third, dominance interactions might be mediated in large part by vocalizations that were not recorded but do often occur at feeding sites [51]. Vampire bats can recognize other individuals at a distance by social calls [77] and possibly by echolocation calls, as reported in other bats [7881]. Such calls might even ensure that higher-ranking and lower-ranking females rarely encounter each other at feeders, such that many clear dominance relationships might be scored as zero.

Fourth, if dominance behaviours in vampire bats are highly context-dependent, then dominance behaviours in the wild and captivity might differ in important ways. In captivity, access to food is much easier than in the natural environment, where a bat must at some risk to itself first make an open wound by biting a much larger host animal that could suddenly attack or move away, and where every wound might also be taken over by other nearby conspecifics [44,45,49,82]. By contrast, the bats in our study did not have to bite hosts, and the multiple blood-filled spouts could always be shared by individuals and were accessible during the entire night, so the benefits of competitive interactions were likely to be greatly reduced relative to the wild.

Given that these limitations would reduce evidence for dominance structures, this suggests that females do have a dominance hierarchy that is underestimated in this study. However, there are also several reasons why one might expect female vampire bats to be exceptionally egalitarian as suggested by our results. The fact that vampire bats regurgitate food to feed their close female associates suggests a lack of competition over food with preferred partners [40,43]. That is, if an individual would give food to another, then there is no reason to use rank to monopolize that food. Socially bonded female vampire bats may also have higher fitness interdependence [83], meaning that each female might benefit from the survival and reproduction of other females in her group. Fitness interdependence involves a correlation between the fitness of different individuals, but it is hard to distinguish cause from effect. Cooperative food-sharing relationships could lead to reduced competition over food among familiar bats, or alternatively, a lack of competition over food between frequent roostmates could facilitate the evolution of cooperative food-sharing relationships.

High fitness interdependence is likely if cooperation occurs ‘locally’ (e.g. blood sharing with familiar bats within the roost) while competition occurs ‘globally’ (e.g. contests over wounds with unfamiliar bats outside the roost). This scenario is consistent with observed patterns of co-roosting and foraging behaviour in the wild [4345,49]. In species with stable groups that move together, highly associated groupmates might also be primary competitors for food, but female vampire bats might be unlikely to repeatedly compete for food with the same individuals with which they roost, groom, and share food. If female vampire bats do not experience high levels of competition within their social networks of familiar females because they compete primarily with bats from foreign groups, then studies on social foraging in vampire bats under natural conditions should reveal that frequent roostmates do not consistently hunt together (as might be expected from past observations [44]), and that most competition over wounds or prey in the wild typically involves unfamiliar vampire bats.

Testing differences in dominance rank by sex in vampire bats will require further study, but social rank in female vampire bats appears to be less clear than among males based on captive [52] and field observations [44,46]. The causes and consequences of social rank are often different for male and female mammals [2]. For instance, in some chimpanzee populations (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii), males challenge others to gain rank whereas females queue [84].

Compared with other group-living mammals, social dominance is understudied in bats [85]. This lack of attention is primarily because competition over food by marked individuals is difficult to observe under natural conditions, but increasing evidence suggests bats do often compete over food [8690], and that competitive or producer–scrounger interactions can occur repeatedly among the same individuals [91]. More comparative data on competitive interactions in bats would be necessary to test whether socioecological models that have been formulated and tested in primates [92,93] could also be applied to bats.

To conclude, we found evidence that female common vampire bats form a dominance hierarchy that appears to be weak and shallow in comparison with females in other well-studied taxa. Female social rank in captive vampire bats is less linear, steep and clear than among primates with female philopatry such as baboons and macaques. This egalitarian social structure among females is consistent with patterns of symmetrical helping and low rates of conflicts among females within the roost, and is possibly due to cooperation and competition occurring among different subsets of bats.

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgements

We thank Rachel Page for logistical support and Tim Fawcett and James Curley for helpful discussions. We thank the editor Oliver Schülke and two anonymous reviewers for feedback that improved the manuscript.

Ethics

Animal procedures were approved by the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute Animal Care and Use Committee (#2015-0915-2018-A9 and #2017-0102-2020) and by the Panamanian Ministry of the Environment (#SE/A-76-16).

Data accessibility

All data and R code can be found on Figshare: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.14043794.v1 [94].

Authors' contributions

R.J.C. collected the data and participated in the study design, data analysis, and writing; L.J.N.B. helped with data analysis and critically revised the manuscript; G.G.C. conceived and coordinated the study and participated in the study design, data analysis and writing. All authors gave final approval for publication and agree to be held accountable for the work performed therein.

Competing interests

We declare we have no competing interests.

Funding

R.J.C. was supported by an Ernst Mayr Short-term Fellowship from the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute. Work by G.G.C. is supported by the National Science Foundation under grant no. 2015928.

References

  • 1.Rosvall KA. 2011. Intrasexual competition in females: evidence for sexual selection? Behav. Ecol. 22, 1131-1140. ( 10.1093/beheco/arr106) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Clutton-Brock TH, Huchard E. 2013. Social competition and selection in males and females. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 368, 20130074. ( 10.1098/rstb.2013.0074) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Rubenstein DR, Lovette IJ. 2009. Reproductive skew and selection on female ornamentation in social species. Nature 462, 786-789. ( 10.1038/nature08614) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Huchard E, English S, Bell MBV, Thavarajah N, Clutton-Brock T. 2016. Competitive growth in a cooperative mammal. Nature 533, 532-534. ( 10.1038/nature17986) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J. 2003. Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science 302, 1231-1234. ( 10.1126/science.1088580) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. 2012. The evolutionary origins of friendship. Ann. Rev. Psych. 63, 153-177. ( 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100337) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Brent LJN, Chang SW, Gariépy J-F, Platt ML. 2014. The neuroethology of friendship. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1316, 1-17. ( 10.1111/nyas.12315) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Brent LJN, Ruiz-Lambides A, Platt ML. 2017. Family network size and survival across the lifespan of female macaques. Proc. R. Soc. B 284, 20170515. ( 10.1098/rspb.2017.0515) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Lukas D, Clutton-Brock T. 2018. Social complexity and kinship in animal societies. Ecol. Lett. 21, 1129-1134. ( 10.1111/ele.13079) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ellis S, Snyder-Mackler N, Ruiz-Lambides A, Platt ML, Brent LJN. 2019. Deconstructing sociality: the types of social connections that predict longevity in a group-living primate. Proc. R. Soc. B 286, 20191991. ( 10.1098/rspb.2019.1991) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bernstein IS, Williams LE. 1983. Ontogenetic changes and the stability of rhesus monkey dominance relationships. Behav. Proc. 8, 379-392. ( 10.1016/0376-6357(83)90025-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Silk JB, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. 1999. The structure of social relationships among female savanna baboons in Moremi Reserve, Botswana. Behaviour 136, 679-703. ( 10.1163/156853999501522) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Altmann J, Alberts SC. 2003. Variability in reproductive success viewed from a life-history perspective in baboons. Amer. J. Hum. Biol. 15, 401-409. ( 10.1002/ajhb.10157) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Blomquist GE, Sade DS, Berard JD. 2011. Rank-related fitness differences and their demographic pathways in semi-free-ranging rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Int. J. Primatol. 32, 193-208. ( 10.1007/s10764-010-9461-z) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Snyder-Mackler N, et al. 2020. Social determinants of health and survival in humans and other animals. Science 368, eaax9553. ( 10.1126/science.aax9553) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Schino G. 2001. Grooming, competition and social rank among female primates: a meta-analysis. Anim. Behav. 62, 265-271. ( 10.1006/anbe.2001.1750) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Silk JB, Altmann J, Alberts SC. 2006. Social relationships among adult female baboons (Papio cynocephalus): I. Variation in the strength of social bonds. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61, 183-195. ( 10.1007/s00265-006-0249-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Thierry B, Aureli F, Nunn CL, Petit O, Abegg C, de Waal FB. 2008. A comparative study of conflict resolution in macaques: insights into the nature of trait covariation. Anim. Behav. 75, 847-860. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.006) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Seyfarth RM, Silk JB, Cheney DL. 2014. Social bonds in female baboons: the interaction between personality, kinship and rank. Anim. Behav. 87, 23-29. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.10.008) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Snyder-Mackler N, Kohn JN, Barreiro LB, Johnson ZP, Wilson ME, Tung J. 2016. Social status drives social relationships in groups of unrelated female rhesus macaques. Anim. Behav. 111, 307-317. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.10.033) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Wu C-F, Liao Z-J, Sueur C, Sha JCM, Zhang J, Zhang P. 2018. The influence of kinship and dominance hierarchy on grooming partner choice in free-ranging Macaca mulatta brevicaudus. Primates 59, 377-384. ( 10.1007/s10329-018-0662-y) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Barrett L, Henzi SP, Weingrill T, Lycett JE, Hill RA. 1999. Market forces predict grooming reciprocity in female baboons. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 266, 665-670. ( 10.1098/rspb.1999.0687) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Barrett L, Gaynor D, Henzi SP. 2002. A dynamic interaction between aggression and grooming reciprocity among female chacma baboons. Anim. Behav. 63, 1047-1053. ( 10.1006/anbe.2002.3008) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Seyfarth RM. 1977. A model of social grooming among adult female monkeys. J. Theor. Biol. 65, 671-698. ( 10.1016/0022-5193(77)90015-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Schino G. 2007. Grooming and agonistic support: a meta-analysis of primate reciprocal altruism. Behav. Ecol. 18, 115-120. ( 10.1093/beheco/arl045) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.de Vries H, Stevens JM, Van Elsacker L, Vervaecke H. 2005. The influence of the steepness of dominance hierarchies on reciprocity and interchange in captive groups of bonobos (Pan paniscus). Behaviour 142, 941-960. ( 10.1163/1568539055010075) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Schino G, Aureli F. 2008. Grooming reciprocation among female primates: a meta-analysis. Biol. Lett. 4, 9-11. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0506) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Jaeggi AV, Stevens JMG, Schaik CPV. 2010. Tolerant food sharing and reciprocity is precluded by despotism among bonobos but not chimpanzees. Am. J. Phys. Anthr. 143, 41-51. ( 10.1002/ajpa.21288) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kaburu SSK, Newton-Fisher NE. 2015. Egalitarian despots: hierarchy steepness, reciprocity and the grooming-trade model in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Anim. Behav. 99, 61-71. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.10.018) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Picard AM, et al. 2020. Why preen others? Predictors of allopreening in parrots and corvids and comparisons to grooming in great apes. Ethology 126, 207-228. ( 10.1111/eth.12999) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Carter GG, Farine DR, Crisp RJ, Vrtilek JK, Ripperger SP, Page RA. 2020. Development of new food-sharing relationships in vampire bats. Curr. Biol. 30, 1275-1279.e3. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2020.01.055) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Meredith RW, et al. 2011. Impacts of the Cretaceous terrestrial revolution and KPg extinction on mammal diversification. Science 334, 521-524. ( 10.1126/science.1211028) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Wilkinson GS, Adams DM. 2019. Recurrent evolution of extreme longevity in bats. Biol. Lett. 15, 20180860. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2018.0860) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Schmidt C. 1988. Reproduction. In Natural history of vampire bats (eds Greenhall AM, Schmidt U), p. 246. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Delpietro H, Russo RG. 2002. Observations of the common vampire bat (Desmodus rotundus) and the hairy-legged vampire bat (Diphylla ecaudata) in captivity. Zeitschrift für Säugetierkunde 67, 65-78. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hermanson JW, Carter GG. 2020. Vampire bats. In Phyllostomid bats, a unique mammalian radiation (eds Fleming T, Davalos L, Mello M), pp. 257-272. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wilkinson GS. 1986. Social grooming in the common vampire bat, Desmodus rotundus. Anim. Behav. 34, 1880-1889. ( 10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80274-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Carter GG, Leffer LL. 2015. Social grooming in bats: are vampire bats exceptional? PLoS ONE 10, e0138430. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0138430) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Narizano H, Carter GG. 2020. Do vampire bats groom others based on need? Behav. Ecol. 31, 107-113. ( 10.1093/beheco/arz165) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Carter GG, Wilkinson GS. 2013. Food sharing in vampire bats: reciprocal help predicts donations more than relatedness or harassment. Proc. R. Soc. B 280, 20122573. ( 10.1098/rspb.2012.2573) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Carter GG, Wilkinson GS. 2015. Social benefits of non-kin food sharing by female vampire bats. Proc. R. Soc. B 282, 20152524. ( 10.1098/rspb.2015.2524) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ripperger SP, et al. 2019. Vampire bats that cooperate in the lab maintain their social networks in the wild. Curr. Biol. 29, 4139-4144.e4. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2019.10.024) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Wilkinson GS. 1984. Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat. Nature 308, 181-184. ( 10.1038/308181a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Wilkinson GS. 1985. The social organization of the common vampire bat: I. Pattern and cause of association. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 17, 111-121. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Delpietro HA, Russo RG, Carter GG, Lord RD, Delpietro GL. 2017. Reproductive seasonality, sex ratio and philopatry in Argentina's common vampire bats. R. Soc. Open Sci. 4, 160959. ( 10.1098/rsos.160959) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Wilkinson GS. 1985. The social organization of the common vampire bat: II. Mating system, genetic structure, and relatedness. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 17, 123-134. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Huguin M, et al. 2018. How social structure drives the population dynamics of the common vampire bat (Desmodus rotundus, Phyllostomidae). J. Hered. 109, 393-404. ( 10.1093/jhered/esx111) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Freitas MB, Queiroz JF, Dias Gomes CI, Collares-Buzato CB, Barbosa HC, Boschero AC, Gonçalves CA, Pinheiro EC. 2013. Reduced insulin secretion and glucose intolerance are involved in the fasting susceptibility of common vampire bats. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 183, 1-6. ( 10.1016/j.ygcen.2012.11.023) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Greenhall AM, Schmidt U, Lopez-Forment W. 1971. Attacking behaviour of the vampire bat, Desmodus rotundus, under field conditions in Mexico. Biotropica 3, 136-141. ( 10.2307/2989817) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Schmidt U, van de Flierdt K. 1973. Intraspecific agonistic behavior of the vampire bat Desmodus rotundus at the feeding site. Zeitschrift fuer Tierpsychologie 32, 139-146. ( 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1973.tb01098.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Sailler H, Schmidt U. 1978. Social calls of the common vampire bat Desmodus rotundus during aggressive behavior at the feeding place in the laboratory. Zeitschrift fuer Saeugetierkunde 43, 249-261. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Park SR. 1988. Dominance relationship in a colony of vampire bat Desmodus rotundus. Korean J. Zool. 31, 243-250. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Park SR. 1991. Development of social structure in a captive colony of the common vampire bat Desmodus rotundus. Ethology 89, 335-341. ( 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1991.tb00378.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Sánchez-Tójar A, Schroeder J, Farine DR. 2018. A practical guide for inferring reliable dominance hierarchies and estimating their uncertainty. J. Anim. Ecol. 87, 594-608. ( 10.1111/1365-2656.12776) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Curley JP. 2016. compete: Analyzing Social Hierarchies: R package version 0.1.
  • 56.de Vries H. 1995. An improved test of linearity in dominance hierarchies containing unknown or tied relationships. Anim. Behav. 50, 1375-1389. ( 10.1016/0003-3472(95)80053-0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Shizuka D, McDonald DB. 2012. A social network perspective on measurements of dominance hierarchies. Anim. Behav. 83, 925-934. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.01.011) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.van Hooff JARAM, Wensing JAB. 1987. Dominance and its behavioral measures in a captive wolf pack. In Man and wolf: advances, issues, and problems in captive wolf research, Perspectives in Vertebrate Science, vol. 4, pp. 219-52. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Dr W Junk Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Vehrencamp SL. 1983. A model for the evolution of despotic versus egalitarian societies. Anim. Behav. 31, 667-682. ( 10.1016/S0003-3472(83)80222-X) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.de Vries H, Stevens JMG, Vervaecke H. 2006. Measuring and testing the steepness of dominance hierarchies. Anim. Behav. 71, 585-592. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.05.015) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.David HA. 1987. Ranking from unbalanced paired-comparison data. Biometrika 74, 432-436. ( 10.1093/biomet/74.2.432) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.de Vries H. 1998. Finding a dominance order most consistent with a linear hierarchy: a new procedure and review. Anim. Behav. 55, 827-843. ( 10.1006/anbe.1997.0708) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Gammell MP, de Vries H, Jennings DJ, Carlin CM, Hayden TJ. 2003. David's score: a more appropriate dominance ranking method than Clutton-Brock et al.’s index. Anim. Behav. 66, 601-605. ( 10.1006/anbe.2003.2226) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Neumann C, Duboscq J, Dubuc C, Ginting A, Irwan AM, Agil M, Widdig A, Engelhardt A. 2011. Assessing dominance hierarchies: validation and advantages of progressive evaluation with Elo-rating. Anim. Behav. 82, 911-921. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.016) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Elo A. 1978. The rating of chess players, past and present. New York, NY: Arco.
  • 66.Albers PC, de Vries H. 2001. Elo-rating as a tool in the sequential estimation of dominance strengths. Anim. Behav. 2, 489-495. ( 10.1006/anbe.2000.1571) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Newton-Fisher NE. 2017. Modeling social dominance: Elo-ratings, prior history, and the intensity of aggression. Int. J. Primatol. 38, 427-447. ( 10.1007/s10764-017-9952-2) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.de Vries H, Appleby MC. 2000. Finding an appropriate order for a hierarchy: a comparison of the I&SI and the BBS methods. Anim. Behav. 59, 239-245. ( 10.1006/anbe.1999.1299) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Oksanen J, et al. 2018. vegan: Community Ecology Package. R package version 2.5-1.
  • 70.Farine DR. 2013. Animal social network inference and permutations for ecologists in R using asnipe. Meth. Ecol. Evol. 4, 1187-1194. ( 10.1111/2041-210X.12121) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Canty A, Ripley B. 2015. boot: Bootstrap R (S-Plus) Functions. R package version 1.
  • 72.Puth M-T, Neuhäuser M, Ruxton GD. 2015. On the variety of methods for calculating confidence intervals by bootstrapping. J. Anim. Ecol. 84, 892-897. ( 10.1111/1365-2656.12382) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Hausfater G, Altmann J, Altmann S. 1982. Long-term consistency of dominance relations among female baboons (Papio cynocephalus). Science 217, 752-755. ( 10.1126/science.217.4561.752) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Sterck EH, Steenbeek R. 1997. Female dominance relationships and food competition in the sympatric Thomas langur and long-tailed macaque. Behaviour 134, 749-774. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Dugatkin LA, Druen M. 2004. The social implications of winner and loser effects. Proc. R. Soc. B 271(Suppl_6), S488-S489. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2004.0235) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Borgeaud C, Bshary R. 2015. Wild vervet monkeys trade tolerance and specific coalitionary support for grooming in experimentally induced conflicts. Curr. Biol. 25, 3011-3016. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.016) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Carter GG, Wilkinson GS. 2016. Common vampire bat contact calls attract past food-sharing partners. Anim. Behav. 116, 45-51. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.03.005) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Kazial KA, Kenny TL, Burnett SC. 2008. Little brown bats (Myotis lucifugus) recognize individual identity of conspecifics using sonar calls. Ethology 114, 469-478. ( 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01483.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Yovel Y, Melcon ML, Franz MO, Denzinger A, Schnitzler HU. 2009. The voice of bats: how greater mouse-eared bats recognize individuals based on their echolocation calls. PLoS Comp Biol. 5, e1000400. ( 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000400) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Voigt-Heucke SL, Taborsky M, Dechmann DKN. 2010. A dual function of echolocation: bats use echolocation calls to identify familiar and unfamiliar individuals. Anim. Behav. 80, 59-67. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.03.025) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Kohles JE, Carter GG, Page RA, Dechmann DKN. 2020. Socially foraging bats discriminate between group members based on search-phase echolocation calls. Behav. Ecol. 31, 1103-1112. ( 10.1093/beheco/araa056) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Delpietro HA. 1989. Case reports on defensive behavior in equine and bovine subjects in response to vocalization of the common vampire bat Desmodus rotundus. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 22, 377-380. ( 10.1016/0168-1591(89)90032-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Roberts G. 2005. Cooperation through interdependence. Anim. Behav. 70, 901-908. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.02.006) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Foerster S, Franz M, Murray CM, Gilby IC, Feldblum JT, Walker KK, Pusey AE. 2016. Chimpanzee females queue but males compete for social status. Sci. Rep. 6, 35404. ( 10.1038/srep35404) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Wilkinson GS, et al. 2019. Kinship, association, and social complexity in bats. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 73, 7. ( 10.1007/s00265-018-2608-1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Lemke TO. 1984. Foraging ecology of the long-nosed bat, Glossophaga soricina, with respect to resource availability. Ecology 65, 538-548. ( 10.2307/1941416) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Barlow KE, Jones G. 1997. Function of pipistrelle social calls: field data and a playback experiment. Anim. Behav. 39, 960-966. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Carter GG, Wilkinson GS. 2013. Cooperation and conflict in the social lives of bats. In Bat evolution, ecology, and conservation (eds Adams R, Pedersen S), pp. 225-242. New York, NY: Springer Science Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Wright GS, Chiu C, Xian W, Wilkinson GS, Moss CF. 2013. Social calls predict foraging success in big brown bats. Curr. Biol. 24, 885-889. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2014.02.058) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Corcoran AJ, Conner WE. 2014. Bats jamming bats: food competition through sonar interference. Science 346, 745-747. ( 10.1126/science.1259512) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Harten L, Matalon Y, Galli N, Navon H, Dor R, Yovel Y. 2018. Persistent producer-scrounger relationships in bats. Sci. Adv. 4, e1603293. ( 10.1126/sciadv.1603293) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Sterck EHM, Watts DP, van Schaik CP. 1997. The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 41, 291-309. ( 10.1007/s002650050390) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Kappeler PM, van Schaik CP. 2002. Evolution of primate social systems. Int. J. Primatol. 23, 707-740. ( 10.1023/A:1015520830318) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Crisp RJ, Carter GG. 2021. Data and R Code for ‘Social dominance and cooperation in female vampire bats'. Figshare. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.14043794.v1) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Citations

  1. Crisp RJ, Carter GG. 2021. Data and R Code for ‘Social dominance and cooperation in female vampire bats'. Figshare. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.14043794.v1) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

All data and R code can be found on Figshare: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.14043794.v1 [94].


Articles from Royal Society Open Science are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES